When I enter the stable and call my horse’s name, she dutifully strolls in from her in/out paddock, sticks her head into the aisleway, and looks my way as if she absolutely understands that she is ‘Farah’ and has decided to acquiesce to my summons. Although this interpretation is comforting, it may be more likely that she has learned from countless repetitions that my arrival and the subsequent call “Farah!” means good things will follow – social interaction, treats, currying, and so on – without any sense of her own unique self or that this unique self is Farah.

Gordon Gallup, the seminal researcher in animal self-awareness, and the inventor of the mirror self-recognition test that measures it, defines self-awareness as “the capacity to become the object of your own attention, [such that] you can begin to think about yourself and use your experience to make inferences about comparable experiences in others” (Gallup & Anderson, 2020, p. 46). Gallup notes that when an animal sees their reflection in the mirror, they have undoubtedly become the object of their own attention; what is more difficult to show is whether they realize that their own behaviour is the source of the behaviour they are seeing in the mirror – the very essence of visual self-recognition.

Children at about the age of two demonstrate self-awareness naturally when they begin to use self-referential language such as I and Me or, or less endearingly, “It’s mine!” They also recognize themselves in a mirror. Gallup’s famous studies in the early 1970s explored whether animals also possessed mirror self-recognition (MSR). Chimpanzees exposed to a mirror, initially responded as if their reflection was another chimp, but gradually these behaviours transitioned to self-directed behaviours – using the mirror to guide self-exploration on body parts not otherwise visible (such as the face, inside the mouth, or their backside).

Advertisement